基本归因错误的文化局限性(4)
[7] Norenzayan A, Nisbett R E. Culture and causal cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2000, (9): 132~135
[8] Miller J G. Culture and the development of everyday social explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1984, 46: 961~978
[9] Morris M W, Peng K. Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1994, 67: 949~971
, 百拇医药
[10] Lee F, Hallahan M, Herzog T. Explaining real life events: How culture and domain shape attributions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1996, (22): 732~741
[11] Krull D S, Loy M H M, Lin J, et al. The fundamental attribution error: Correspondence bias in independent and interdependent cultures. Paper presented at the 13th Congress of the International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology, Montreal, Quebec, Canada, 1996
, 百拇医药
[12] Fiske A P, Kitayama S, Markus H R, Nisbett R E. The cultural matrix of social psychology. In: Gilbert D T, Fiske S T, Lindzey G. The Handbook of Social Psychology. fourth edition. Boston: The McGraw-Hill, 1998. volume II, 915~981
[13] Gilbert D T, Jones E E. Perceiver-induced constraint: Interpretations of self-generated reality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1986, 50: 269~280
[14] Choi I, Nisbett, R E. Situational salience and cultural differences in the correspondence bias and in the actor-observer bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1998, (24): 949~960
, http://www.100md.com
[15] Miyamoto Y, Kitayama S. Cultural Variation in Correspondence Bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2002, 83 (5): 1239~1248
[16] Nisbett R. E. The geography of thought: How Asians and westerners think differently…and why. New York: Free Press, 2004
[17] Nisbett R E, Peng K, Choi I, Norenzayan A. Culture and systems of thought: Holistic vs. analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 2001, 108: 291~310
, 百拇医药
[18] Menon T, Morris M W, Chiu C, Hong Y. Culture and the construal of agency. Journal of personality and social psychology, 1999, 76 (5): 1188~1199
[19] Hong Y, Chiu C, Kung T. Bringing culture out in front: Effects of cultural meaning system activation on social cognition. In: K Leung, Y Kashima, U Kim, S Yamaguchi, eds. Progress in Asian Social Psychology, 1. Singapore: Wiley, 1997. 135~146
[20] Markus H R, Kitayama S. Cultural and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 1991, 98: 224~253
, 百拇医药
[21] Choi I, Dalal R, Chu K, Park H. Culture and Judgment of Causal Relevance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2003, 84(1): 46~59
[22] Langdridge D, Butt T. The fundamental attribution error: A phenomenalogical critique. British Journal of Social Psychology, 2004, 43: 357~369
[23] Cartwright D. Contemporary social psychology in historical perspective. Social Psychology Quarterly, 1979, 42: 82~93
[ 上 页 ], http://www.100md.com(李 陈 陈午晴)
[8] Miller J G. Culture and the development of everyday social explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1984, 46: 961~978
[9] Morris M W, Peng K. Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1994, 67: 949~971
, 百拇医药
[10] Lee F, Hallahan M, Herzog T. Explaining real life events: How culture and domain shape attributions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1996, (22): 732~741
[11] Krull D S, Loy M H M, Lin J, et al. The fundamental attribution error: Correspondence bias in independent and interdependent cultures. Paper presented at the 13th Congress of the International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology, Montreal, Quebec, Canada, 1996
, 百拇医药
[12] Fiske A P, Kitayama S, Markus H R, Nisbett R E. The cultural matrix of social psychology. In: Gilbert D T, Fiske S T, Lindzey G. The Handbook of Social Psychology. fourth edition. Boston: The McGraw-Hill, 1998. volume II, 915~981
[13] Gilbert D T, Jones E E. Perceiver-induced constraint: Interpretations of self-generated reality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1986, 50: 269~280
[14] Choi I, Nisbett, R E. Situational salience and cultural differences in the correspondence bias and in the actor-observer bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1998, (24): 949~960
, http://www.100md.com
[15] Miyamoto Y, Kitayama S. Cultural Variation in Correspondence Bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2002, 83 (5): 1239~1248
[16] Nisbett R. E. The geography of thought: How Asians and westerners think differently…and why. New York: Free Press, 2004
[17] Nisbett R E, Peng K, Choi I, Norenzayan A. Culture and systems of thought: Holistic vs. analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 2001, 108: 291~310
, 百拇医药
[18] Menon T, Morris M W, Chiu C, Hong Y. Culture and the construal of agency. Journal of personality and social psychology, 1999, 76 (5): 1188~1199
[19] Hong Y, Chiu C, Kung T. Bringing culture out in front: Effects of cultural meaning system activation on social cognition. In: K Leung, Y Kashima, U Kim, S Yamaguchi, eds. Progress in Asian Social Psychology, 1. Singapore: Wiley, 1997. 135~146
[20] Markus H R, Kitayama S. Cultural and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 1991, 98: 224~253
, 百拇医药
[21] Choi I, Dalal R, Chu K, Park H. Culture and Judgment of Causal Relevance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2003, 84(1): 46~59
[22] Langdridge D, Butt T. The fundamental attribution error: A phenomenalogical critique. British Journal of Social Psychology, 2004, 43: 357~369
[23] Cartwright D. Contemporary social psychology in historical perspective. Social Psychology Quarterly, 1979, 42: 82~93
[ 上 页 ], http://www.100md.com(李 陈 陈午晴)