Mechanisms of Fasting and Postprandial Hyperglycemia in People With Im
http://www.100md.com
《糖尿病学杂志》
1 Division of Endocrinology, Diabetes, Metabolism and Nutrition, Mayo Clinic College of Medicine, Rochester, Minnesota
2 Department of Electronics and Informatics, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
DI, disposition index; EGP, endogenous glucose production; IFG, impaired fasting glucose; IGT, impaired glucose tolerance; NFG, normal fasting glucose; NGT, normal glucose tolerance; OGTT, oral glucose tolerance test
ABSTRACT
Thirty-two subjects with impaired fasting glucose (IFG) and 28 subjects with normal fasting glucose (NFG) ingested a labeled meal and 75 g glucose (oral glucose tolerance test) on separate occasions. Fasting glucose, insulin, and C-peptide were higher (P < 0.05) in subjects with IFG than in those with NFG, whereas endogenous glucose production (EGP) did not differ, indicating hepatic insulin resistance. EGP was promptly suppressed, and meal glucose appearance comparably increased following meal ingestion in both groups. In contrast, glucose disappearance (Rd) immediately after meal ingestion was lower (P < 0.001) in subjects with IFG/impaired glucose tolerance (IGT) and IFG/diabetes but did not differ in subjects with IFG/normal glucose tolerance (NGT) or NFG/NGT. Net insulin action (Si) and insulin-stimulated glucose disposal (Si) were reduced (P < 0.001, ANOVA) in subjects with NFG/IGT, IFG/IGT, and IFG/diabetes but did not differ in subjects with NFG/NGT or IFG/NGT. Defective insulin secretion also contributed to lower postprandial Rd since disposition indexes were lower (P < 0.001, ANOVA) in subjects with NFG/IGT, IFG/IGT, and IFG/diabetes but did not differ in subjects with NFG/NGT and IFG/NGT. We conclude that postprandial hyperglycemia in individuals with early diabetes is due to lower rates of glucose disappearance rather than increased meal appearance or impaired suppression of EGP, regardless of their fasting glucose. In contrast, insulin secretion, action, and the pattern of postprandial turnover are essentially normal in individuals with isolated IFG.
Individuals with impaired fasting glucose (IFG) have a 20–30% chance of developing diabetes over the next 5–10 years (1–3). The risk is even greater if they have combined IFG and impaired glucose tolerance (IGT). Furthermore, IFG and IGT are associated with increased risk of cardiovascular events (4,5). Therefore, the pathogenesis of IFG alone or in combination with IGT has engendered considerable interest. Glucose concentration begins to increase when glucose appearance exceeds glucose disappearance and continues to increase until these two rates are once again equal. In the fasting state, glucose appearance is determined by the rate of glucose release from the liver with perhaps a small contribution by the kidney. Together, these processes are referred to as endogenous glucose production (EGP). The situation is more complex following food ingestion when glucose appearance equals the sum of EGP and the rate of appearance of the ingested glucose (6). There is currently limited data as to the contribution of these processes to fasting hyperglycemia (7,8) and no data regarding the regulation of postprandial glucose metabolism in individuals with IFG. The latter is of particular interest since while most individuals with IFG also have either IGT or diabetes, some have normal glucose tolerance (NGT). On the other hand, some individuals with normal fasting glucose (NFG) have IGT.
When considered in the light of the prevailing glucose and insulin concentration, EGP is increased in individuals with mild and severe type 2 diabetes (9). To our knowledge, Weyer et al. (7) are the only investigators who have measured EGP in individuals with IFG. In those studies, fasting EGP was increased in Pima Indians with IFG, regardless of whether they had NGT or IGT. In contrast, fasting glucose production was not elevated in Pima Indians with IGT and NFG concentrations. However, since fasting insulin concentrations were elevated in those subjects, this implies the presence of hepatic insulin resistance. On the other hand, since glucose and insulin suppress glucose production (10–13) and enhanced insulin secretion can potentially compensate for a defect in insulin action (14,15), hepatic insulin resistance does not necessarily mean that excessive EGP is the cause of postprandial hyperglycemia in individuals with IFG and/or IGT. Conversely, effective compensation via these mechanisms could normalize postprandial suppression of EGP, thereby enabling some individuals with IFG to maintain normal postprandial glucose concentrations.
To our knowledge, the postprandial rate of meal glucose appearance has not been measured in individuals with IFG. However, Sacca et al. (16) reported that splanchnic glucose uptake during intravenous glucose infusion was greater in six subjects with combined IFG/IGT than in seven normoglycemic control subjects, implying enhanced rather than decreased hepatic glucose extraction. Unfortunately, these data are difficult to interpret since glucose concentrations were far higher in subjects with IFG than in control subjects, and glucose is a potent stimulus of hepatic glucose uptake (13,17). Therefore, the relative contribution of alterations in meal-derived glucose appearance, EGP, or glucose disappearance to the regulation of postprandial glucose concentrations in individuals with IFG is presently not known.
To address these questions, meal glucose appearance, EGP, and glucose disappearance were measured in 32 subjects with IFG before and after ingestion of a mixed meal using a validated triple-tracer approach (18). Insulin secretion and action were concurrently measured using the C-peptide (19,20) and "oral" minimal models (21–25) in order to gain insight regarding the etiology of alterations (if observed) in postprandial glucose metabolism. Results were compared with those observed in 28 control subjects with NFG. In addition, to gain insight regarding the heterogeneity of glucose tolerance in individuals with NFG or IFG, the subjects were subdivided according to whether they also fulfilled oral glucose tolerance test (OGTT) criteria for IGT or diabetes.
RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODS
After approval from the Mayo Institutional Review Board, 32 subjects (17 women and 15 men) with IFG and 28 subjects (17 women and 11 men) with NFG gave informed written consent to participate in the study. All subjects were Caucasian, in good health, at a stable weight, and did not engage in regular vigorous physical exercise. At the time of study, subjects were on no medications other than a stable dose of thyroid hormone, low-dose aspirin, hydroxymethylglutaryl-CoA reductase inhibitors, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor antidepressants, or antihypertensives, which are metabolically neutral (e.g., no ACE inhibitors or -blockers).
All subjects were instructed to follow a weight-maintenance diet containing 55% carbohydrate, 30% fat, and 15% protein for at least 3 days before the study date. Fasting plasma glucose concentration was measured after an overnight fast on two separate occasions at least 1 week apart. Subjects whose average fasting glucose level was <5.2 mmol/l or between 5.6 and 7.0 mmol/l were selected for the study and referred to as having NFG or IFG, respectively. Subjects with a fasting glucose between 5.2 and 5.6 mmol/l were excluded from the study, since despite having glucose concentrations within the normal range, previous studies have shown that such individuals have an 8% risk of developing diabetes within the next 10 years and, therefore, possibly represent an early form of IFG (1–3). Eligible subjects were then admitted to the Mayo General Clinical Research Center on two subsequent occasions at 1700 the evening before the study and ate a standard 10 kcal/kg meal (55% carbohydrate, 30% fat, and 15% protein) between 1830 and 1900. No additional food was eaten until the next morning. On one occasion, subjects ingested 75 g glucose after a 12-h overnight fast. Based on these results, subjects with either NFG or IFG were subclassified as having NFG/NGT (2-h plasma glucose <7.8 mmol/l), NFG/IGT (2-h plasma glucose between 7.8 and 11.1 mmol/l), IFG/IGT, or IFG/diabetes (2-h plasma glucose >11.1 mmol/l).
On another occasion, subjects ingested a labeled mixed meal as previously described (18). In brief, an 18-gauge cannula was inserted at 0600 into a forearm vein for tracer infusions. Another 18-gauge cannula was inserted in a retrograde fashion in a dorsal hand vein of the opposite arm, and the hand was placed in a heated box (55°C) to enable sampling of arterialized venous blood. A primed (12 mg/kg) continuous (0.12 mg · kg–1 · min–1) infusion of [6,6-2H2]glucose (MassTrace, Woburn, MA) started at 0700. At time 0, i.e., 1000, subjects ingested a standard mixed meal within 15 min, which consisted of three scrambled eggs, 55 g Canadian bacon (or 47 g steak), and Jell-O containing 75 g glucose that was enriched (to 4%) with [1-13C]glucose, as previously described (18,26). An intravenous infusion of [6-3H]glucose was started at the same time and infused in a pattern that minimized the change in the ratio of plasma tracer (i.e., [6-3H]glucose) to plasma meal tracee (i.e., [1-13C]glucose). In addition, the [6,6-2H2]glucose infusion was varied in a manner mimicking the anticipated pattern of change of EGP, thereby also minimizing the change in the ratio of plasma tracer (i.e., [6,6-2H2]glucose) to plasma tracee (i.e., concentration of endogenous plasma glucose), as previously described (18,27).
Analytical techniques.
Plasma samples were placed on ice, centrifuged at 4°C, separated, and stored at –20°C until the assay was complete. Glucose concentrations were measured using a glucose oxidase method (Yellow Springs Instruments, Yellow Springs, OH). Plasma insulin was measured using a chemiluminescence assay with reagents obtained from Beckman (Access Assay; Beckman, Chaska, MN). Plasma glucagon and C-peptide were measured by radioimmunoassay using reagents supplied by Linco Research (St. Louis, MO). Body composition was measured using dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry (DPX Scanner; Lunar, Madison, WI) and computerized absorption tomography with cuts at L2/3 and T11/12 to determine percent body fat and visceral fat. Plasma [6,6-2H2]glucose and [1-13C]glucose enrichments were measured using gas chromatography–mass spectrometry (Thermoquest, San Jose, CA) to simultaneously monitor the C-1, C-2, and C-3 to C-6 fragments, as described by Beylot et al. (28), and [6-3H]glucose specific activity by liquid-scintillation counting following deproteinization and passage over anion and cation exchange columns.
Calculations.
The systemic rates of meal appearance (RaMEAL), EGP, and glucose disappearance (Rd) were calculated using Radziuk’s two-compartment model (29), as previously described (18). In brief, RaMEAL was calculated by multiplying the rate of appearance of [1-13C]glucose (obtained from the infusion rate of [6-3H]glucose and the clamped plasma ratio of [6-3H]glucose and [1-13C]glucose) by the meal enrichment (i.e., the ratio of total glucose to tracer in the meal). EGP was calculated from the infusion rate of [6,62H2]glucose and the clamped plasma ratio of [6,62H2]glucose to endogenous glucose concentration. Glucose disappearance was calculated by subtracting the change in glucose mass from the overall rate of glucose appearance (i.e., RaMEAL + EGP).
Insulin sensitivity (Si), which measures the overall effect of insulin to stimulate glucose disposal and inhibit glucose production, was estimated from plasma glucose and insulin concentrations using the oral glucose minimal model (24,25). The model assumes that insulin action on glucose production and disposal emanates from a compartment remote from plasma, which is usually identified with the interstitium. Similarly, the selective effect of insulin on glucose disposal (Si) was estimated from oral-ingested glucose tracer and insulin concentration by using the labeled oral glucose minimal model (23).
-Cell responsivity indexes were estimated from plasma glucose and C-peptide concentrations measured during the test by using the oral C-peptide minimal model (19), incorporating age-associated changes in C-peptide kinetics as measured by Van Cauter et al. (30). The model assumes that insulin secretion is made up of two components. The dynamic component is likely to represent secretion of promptly releasable insulin and is proportional to the rate of increase of glucose concentration through a parameter, Phidynamic, which defines the dynamic responsivity index. The static component is derived from the provision of new insulin to the releasable pool and is characterized by a static index and by a delay time constant, T. The meaning of Phistatic and T can be made clear with reference to a response to an above-basal step increase of glucose; provision tends, with time constant T, toward a steady state, which is linearly related to the glucose step size through parameter Phistatic. To determine if insulin secretion indexes were appropriate for the prevailing level of insulin action, the disposition indexes (DIs) DItotal, DIdynamic, and DIstatic were calculated by multiplying Phitotal, Phidynamic, and Phistatic, respectively, by Si.
Values from –30 to 0 min were averaged and considered as basal. The area above basal was calculated using the trapezoidal rule. Parameters of all models were estimated by using the SAAMII software (31). Measurement errors were assumed to be independent and Gaussian, with zero mean and variance for glucose and tracer glucose (22) and for C-peptide (32).
Statistical analysis.
All data are presented as means ± SE. Rates of glucose turnover are expressed as micromoles per kilogram lean body mass. Two sample comparisons between subjects with IFG and NFG were made using Student’s t test or rank-sum test for data that were nonnormally distributed. Analyses among the NFG and IFG subgroups were made using ANOVA followed, where appropriate, by Student’s two-tailed nonpaired t test. A P value <0.05 was considered statistically significant.
RESULTS
By design, the fasting plasma glucose levels at screening were higher (P < 0.001) in subjects with IFG than in those with NFG/NGT (Table 1). Age, lean body mass, and body fat did not differ statistically between groups. On the other hand, BMI and visceral fat were greater (P < 0.05) in subjects with versus NFG/NGT. None of the above parameters differed in subjects with IFG with or without IGT or diabetes or in subjects with NFG with or without IGT. Sixty percent of the subjects with IFG/IGT and 50% of subjects with IFG/diabetes had a history of diabetes in a first-degree relative. No subject with IFG/NGT had a family history of diabetes. By selection, none of the subjects with NFG had a family history of diabetes.
Plasma glucose, insulin, C-peptide, and glucagon concentrations.
Fasting plasma glucose concentrations were higher (P < 0.001) in the total group with IFG than in subjects with NFG/NGT (Fig. 1) and increased to a higher peak (P < 0.002) following meal ingestion (11.9 ± 0.4 vs. 9.9 ± 0.2 mmol/l). On the other hand, the glucose area above basal did not differ statistically between these two groups. When analyzed according to OGTT status, fasting plasma glucose concentrations were higher (P < 0.001) in subjects with IFG/diabetes but did not differ in the those with IFG/NGT and IFG/IGT (Fig. 2). Fasting glucose also did not differ in the subjects with NFG/IGT and NFG/NGT. Following meal ingestion, glucose concentration increased to a higher (P < 0.001) peak in the subjects with IFG/IGT (11.2 ± 0.4 mmol/l) and IFG/diabetes (14.5 ± 0.4 mmol/l) compared with subjects with NFG/NGT. On the other hand, peak glucose concentration did not differ in subjects with IFG/NGT and NFG/NGT (10.6 ± 0.6 vs. 9.9 ± 0.2 mmol/l, respectively), resulting in a lower (P < 0.05) postprandial increment in glucose in subjects with IFG/NGT. Peak glucose concentrations also did not differ in the subjects with NFG/IGT and NFG/NGT (10.4 ± 0.4 vs. 9.9 ± 0.2 mmol/l, respectively); however, it took longer for glucose concentrations to return to preprandial levels in the former, resulting in a greater (P < 0.05) glucose area above basal in the subjects with NFG/IGT. Glucose area above basal was also greater (P < 0.001) in the subjects with IFG/diabetes than in those with NFG/NGT.
Fasting plasma insulin concentrations were higher (P < 0.05) in the total group with IFG than in the subjects with NFG/NGT (Fig. 1) but rose to a comparable peak after meal ingestion (628 ± 45 vs. 531 ± 86 pmol/l, respectively) (Table 2). However, the time to peak was longer (P < 0.01) in the total group with IFG than in the subjects with NFG/NGT (88 ± 7 vs. 57 ± 6 min, respectively). Subgroups analysis (Fig. 2) indicated that the higher fasting plasma insulin concentrations in the total group with IFG were primarily due to higher insulin concentrations in subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes (P < 0.05 vs. NFG/NGT) since insulin concentrations did not differ among the other groups. The insulin area above basal did not differ among the groups during the 1st hour after meal ingestion (i.e., when glucose concentrations were diverging in the subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes). The insulin area above basal during the entire 6-h observation also did not differ among the subjects with NGT and IGT but was higher (P < 0.05) in the subjects with IFG/diabetes than in those with NFG/NGT. However, the time to peak insulin concentration was longer (P < 0.01) in the subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes (92 ± 8 and 115 ± 11 min, respectively) than in those with NFG/NGT.
Fasting plasma C-peptide concentrations also were higher (P < 0.05) in the total group with IFG than in the subjects with NFG/NGT and rose to a comparable peak after meal ingestion (3.4 ± 0.2 vs. 3.0 ± 0.3 nmol/l, respectively). As with insulin, the higher fasting C-peptide concentrations in the total group with IFG were primarily due to higher C-peptide concentrations in subjects with IFG/diabetes (P < 0.001 vs. NFG/NGT). On the other hand, the C-peptide area above basal during the first 60 min after meal ingestion, and during the entire 6 h of observation, did not differ among subjects with NGT and IGT. In contrast, the C-peptide area above basal over the 6 h of observation was higher (P < 0.05) in subjects with IFG/diabetes than in those with NFG/NGT.
Fasting plasma glucagon concentrations were higher (P < 0.02) in the total group with IFG than in the subjects with NFG/NGT before meal ingestion (75 ± 4 vs. 57 ± 4 pg/ml, respectively) but did not differ after meal ingestion. While fasting plasma glucagon concentrations were somewhat higher in the subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes compared with those with NFG/NGT, the differences were not significant. Plasma glucagon concentrations remained constant or tended to fall immediately after meal ingestion in all subgroups, then subsequently rose as glucose concentrations fell back toward preprandial values.
Meal glucose appearance, EGP, and glucose disappearance.
The systemic rate of appearance of meal-derived glucose reached a comparable peak within 30–60 min after meal ingestion in subjects with IFG and NFG/NGT. While the area above basal did not differ among groups, the area above baseline over the 6 h of observation tended (P = 0.08) to be lower in the subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes compared with the subjects with NFG/NGT.
Despite increased fasting glucose and insulin concentrations, fasting EGP did not differ in subjects with IFG and NFG. Subgroup analysis indicated that EGP also did not differ in subjects with or without IGT and was slightly higher (P = 0.08) in subjects with IFG/diabetes. In addition, postprandial suppression of endogenous glucose (area below basal) following meal ingestion also did not differ between groups.
Glucose disappearance before meal ingestion also did not differ in the total goup with IFG and the subjects with NFG/NGT or in the various subgroups. On the other hand, the increment above basal during the 1st hour after meal ingestion was smaller (P < 0.002) in the total group with IFG than in the subjects with NFG/NGT (Fig. 3). The blunted increase in glucose disappearance immediately after meal ingestion was primarily due to a smaller increase in the subjects with IFG/diabetes (P < 0.001) and IFG/IGT (P < 0.001) (Fig. 4). The increase in glucose disappearance during the 1st hour after meal ingestion tended to be lower (P = 0.08) in the subjects with NFG/IGT than in those with NFG/NGT. Glucose disappearance reached comparable rates in all groups by 2 h after meal ingestion and was slightly higher in the subjects with IGT and diabetes than in those with NGT, resulting in a comparable total area above basal during the 6 h of observation.
Indexes of insulin action.
Net insulin action (Si) and the ability of insulin to stimulate glucose uptake (Si) were measured following meal ingestion with the respective unlabeled and labeled oral minimal models (Fig. 5). Si (P < 0.005) and Si (P < 0.001) were lower in the total group with IFG than in the subjects with NFG/NGT. Subgroup analysis indicated that Si and Si were lower (P < 0.005) in subjects with NFG/IGT, IFG/IGT, and IFG/diabetes than in those with NFG/NGT. Si and Si were also lower (P < 0.05) in the subjects with IFG/diabetes than in those with IFG/NGT. On the other hand, while Si and Si were numerically lower in the subjects with IFG/NGT versus those with NFG/NGT, the differences were not significant.
Indexes of insulin secretion.
Indexes of insulin secretion were measured following meal ingestion with the C-peptide minimal model, which enabled concurrent assessment with insulin action (Fig. 6). The overall response to glucose (Phitotal), the response to a change in glucose (Phidynamic), and the response to a given glucose level (Phistatic) did not differ in the total group with IFG and those with NFG/NGT. However, when DIs were calculated to determine if insulin secretion was appropriate for the prevailing level of insulin action, DItotal (P < 0.01), DIstatic (P = 0.01), and DIdynamic (P < 0.001) were all lower in the total group with IFG than in those with NFG/NGT.
Phidynamic did not differ among the subjects with NFG/NGT, IFG/NGT, and NFG/IGT but tended to be lower in those with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes. Phitotal and Phistatic also did not differ among the groups. However, the time required to reach Phistatic (37 ± 5 vs. 14 ± 2 min) was longer (P < 0.001) in those with IFG/diabetes than in those with NFG/NGT but did not differ among the other groups. On the other hand, when the appropriateness of insulin secretion for the prevailing level of insulin action was considered, DItotal, DIdynamic, and DIstaticall were lower in subjects with NFG/IGT (P < 0.05), IFG/IGT (P < 0.01), and IFG/diabetes (P < 0.001) than in those with NFG/NGT. Furthermore, DItotal and DIstatic were also lower in subjects with NFG/IGT (P < 0.01), IFG/IGT (P < 0.005), and IFG/diabetes (P < 0.001) compared with those with IFG/NGT, and DIdynamic was lower (P < 0.01) in subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes than in those with IFG/NGT. While DItotal, DIdynamic, and DIstatic did not differ in subjects with NFG/IGT and IFG/IGT, they tended to be lower (P = 0.06) in subjects with IFG/diabetes compared with those with NFG/IGT. On the other hand, none of the DIs differed in subjects with IFG/NFG and NFG/NGT.
DISCUSSION
People with pre-diabetes (i.e., IFG and/or IGT) are at increased risk of developing overt diabetes (1–3). The present study indicates that fasting EGP is inappropriately increased and glucose disappearance is inappropriately decreased in individuals with IFG when considered in light of the higher prevailing glucose and insulin concentrations. It therefore appears that both abnormalities contribute to IFG. On the other hand, EGP is promptly suppressed in individuals with IFG and/or IGT after meal ingestion. The rate of meal appearance also did not differ among groups, indicating that excessive glucose absorption and/or decreased hepatic glucose uptake does not cause postprandial hyperglycemia in individuals with pre-diabetes. In contrast, postprandial glucose disposal was decreased in individuals with IGT or diabetes, with the decrease being most evident in individuals who also had IFG. Both the defects in insulin secretion and the ability of insulin to stimulate glucose uptake contributed to the lower rates of disposal, since both were impaired in individuals with IGT or diabetes. On the other hand, postprandial EGP, meal appearance, glucose disposal, insulin secretion, and insulin action were all normal in individuals with isolated IFG, implying a set point abnormality with an intact -cell response to food ingestion. Taken together, these data indicate that both the defects in insulin secretion and action contribute to postprandial hyperglycemia. They also indicate that the pattern of postprandial glucose metabolism is essentially normal in individuals with isolated IFG, perhaps presaging a lower risk of progression to overt diabetes.
Rates and disappearance of EGP did not differ in subjects with IFG and NFG before meal ingestion. However, since both glucose and insulin concentrations were higher in the former than in the latter, fasting EGP was inappropriately elevated and disappearance inappropriately reduced. Subgroup analysis indicated that the increase in fasting glucose production was most evident in subjects with the worst glucose tolerance (i.e., subjects with IFG/diabetes). This observation supports prior reports by Weyer et al. (7) and Lillioja et al. (33) that Pima Indians with IFG have both hepatic and extrahepatic insulin resistance. Of interest, fasting glucagon concentrations were higher in subjects with IFG than in subjects with NFG/NGT. The increase in fasting glucagon appeared to be most evident in the subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes; however, the number of subjects in these subgroups was relatively small, and the differences were not significant. Nevertheless, these data suggest that glucagon may contribute to increased fasting glucose concentrations in individuals with IFG.
EGP rapidly suppressed in all groups, following meal ingestion, presumably reflecting the combined suppressive effects of the concurrent increases in glucose and insulin (9–13). While the rate of suppression during the 1st hour after food ingestion tended to be slower in subjects with IFG/diabetes, it did not differ significantly from that observed in subjects with NFG. This pattern is not dissimilar to the situation observed in individuals with overt diabetes who also ultimately suppress EGP to normal following food ingestion (34–37). However, since the preprandial rates are increased and defects in insulin secretion are more marked in individuals with "severe" diabetes, the time to suppression is delayed and excess amounts of glucose enter circulation. Taken together, these data lend additional support to the concept that hepatic insulin resistance occurs early in the evolution of type 2 diabetes. On the other hand, failure to appropriately suppress EGP is not the cause of postprandial hyperglycemia in individuals with IGT or early diabetes.
The systemic rate of appearance of the ingested glucose did not differ in subjects with IFG and NFG and, if anything, tended to be lower in the subgroups with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes, perhaps reflecting increased hepatic glucose uptake due to higher portal glucose concentrations. Therefore, increased meal glucose appearance was not the cause of the higher postprandial glucose concentrations in any of the groups. In contrast, postprandial glucose disappearance was lower in subjects with IFG immediately after meal ingestion, particularly during the 1st hour when the excessive rise in glucose occurred in subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes. Of note, glucose disappearance in subjects with NFG/IGT during the 1st hour after meal ingestion only slightly reduced, perhaps accounting for the fact that the peak glucose concentration in this group did not differ from that in subjects with NFG/NGT. However, it took longer for glucose to return to preprandial concentrations, resulting in higher glucose concentrations at 2 h in subjects with NFG/IGT. Since insulin secretion was relatively intact in these individuals, whereas insulin action was markedly decreased, this postprandial pattern of change in glucose concentrations (and the OGTT pattern, since that is why subjects in this group were classified as having IGT) is consistent with previous reports indicating that a delay and decrease in early insulin secretion results in a higher peak glucose concentration, whereas a defect in insulin action results in an increased duration of hyperglycemia (38,39). Of interest, subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes had both a higher peak glucose concentration and a more prolonged duration of hyperglycemia. This occurred since they were both insulin resistant and had a severe defect in their ability to secrete insulin in response to the rapid increase in glucose, which occurred immediately after meal ingestion.
Combined use of the oral minimal and C-peptide models enabled indexes of insulin action and insulin secretion to be simultaneously measured under physiologic conditions in the same individual. This is important since insulin action is context dependent in that it differs depending on how much insulin is given and the pattern in which it is given (40–42). Therefore, the observation that net insulin action (Si) was decreased in subjects with IFG following meal ingestion is reassuring and consistent with previous studies that have measured net insulin action in the fasting state using the homeostasis model assessment method (43–48), following intravenous glucose injection or during insulin infusion (7,49). The labeled oral minimal model established that the decrease in net insulin action was due, at least in part, to a decrease in the ability of insulin to stimulate glucose uptake (Si). Subgroup analysis indicated that this was primarily due to reduced insulin action in subjects with IGT or diabetes, irrespective of their fasting glucose concentration. On the other hand, insulin action did not differ significantly in subjects with NFG/NGT and IFG/NGT.
DIdynamic, which assesses the appropriateness of insulin secretion in response to a change in glucose, was impaired in all of the subjects with IGT, with the severity of the defect increasing as glucose tolerance deteriorated. To our knowledge, the only other study examining a similar aspects of insulin secretion is that of Ferrannini et al. (50). The authors used a model to evaluate insulin secretion in individuals with IGT during an OGTT, which was similar to the model used in the present experiments. They reported that while the static response to insulin was decreased in their subjects with IGT, the dynamic response to glucose was intact. However, this discrepancy is likely more apparent than real since the appropriateness of the dynamic response was not considered in light of the 50% reduction in insulin action in subjects with IGT. Furthermore, their IGT group contained subjects with both NFG and IFG. As is evident in the present studies, the impairment in DIdynamic is more marked in individuals with both IFG and IGT than in those with IGT alone, which likely accounts for the higher peak postprandial glucose concentrations in the former than in the latter (see Fig. 2).
The static response was decreased in all subjects with IGT. This observation is consistent with the previous report of Ferraninni et al. (50), which showed that the static response to glucose is decreased during an OGTT in individuals with IGT. The static response to glucose evaluates the amount of insulin that is secreted at any given level of glucose and therefore has been referred to as "glucose sensitivity." Since this response occurs throughout the entire 6 h of study, it presumably is influenced by insulin synthesis and processing, as well as more distal steps in the insulin secretion pathway. The model used in the current experiments indicates that there was a delay between the time when glucose reaches a given level and when the static response achieves a steady state. This time (t in the model) averaged 10 min in subjects with NFG/NGT and tended to be slightly increased (15 min) in subjects with IGT. In contrast, it was markedly prolonged in subjects with IFG/diabetes, averaging 35 min. Of interest, in vitro studies suggest that 8–10 min are required for insulin granules in the storage pool to move to the plasma membrane, dock, and become primed for exocytosis (50–53). It is interesting to speculate that the activation of this process is required for the static response to glucose to achieve a steady state and that the development of diabetes either is exacerbated by or causes a delay in the rate at which insulin granules in the storage pool are primed for secretion.
The results of subjects with IFG/NGT are particularly intriguing. Despite fasting hyperglycemia, postprandial glucose concentrations were virtually identical to those in subjects with NFG/NGT. Since preprandial glucose concentrations were higher, the postprandial increment in glucose concentration was lower than in subjects with NFG/NGT (Fig. 2 and Table 2). This was not due to reduced glucose absorption or increased hepatic glucose uptake, since the rate of appearance of ingested glucose was virtually identical in both groups, as were postprandial changes in EGP and disposal. Insulin action was slightly, but not significantly, lower in subjects with IFG/NGT compared with those with NFG/NGT, perhaps because of greater visceral adiposity in the former. Indexes of insulin secretion including the dynamic and static responses to glucose were normal whether assessed as actual responses (Fig. 6) or when corrected for the degree of insulin resistance by calculating DIs (Fig. 7). Preprandial rates of EGP and disappearance did not differ in subjects with IFG/NGT and NFG/NGT. However, in contrast to subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes, fasting insulin concentrations were not increased, making it difficult to invoke insulin resistance as the cause of fasting hyperglycemia. On the other hand, since glucose production and disappearance did not differ in subjects with IFG/NGT and NFG/NGT and since, as noted above, even small increases in glucose concentrations result in suppression of glucose production (14,15), this implies that glucose concentration continued to increase until what has recently been referred to as "allostasis" (54) was again achieved at a higher glucose concentration.
The observations that fasting insulin concentration was normal, despite a higher fasting glucose concentration, and that insulin secretion in response to a meal-related rise in glucose concentration was also normal argue for a higher "set point" in the subjects with IFG/NGT. A decrease in -cell glucokinase activity in subjects with IFG/NGT could provide an explanation for this pattern of response (39). However, none of subjects with IFG/NGT had a family history of diabetes, which is in contrast to the subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes, whereas 50–60% of the subjects had a first-degree relative with diabetes. Since subjects with a defect in glucokinase activity generally have a family history of diabetes, the lack of family history in the subjects with IFG/NGT reduces the likelihood of such a defect. Nevertheless, the cause and potential consequences (e.g., relative risk of subsequently developing diabetes) of elevated fasting but normal postprandial glucose concentrations in these individuals clearly warrant further study.
The present study has certain limitations. The subjects were healthy, had no history of vascular disease, and were only modestly obese (BMI 31 kg/m2). More marked abnormalities in insulin secretion, action, and postprandial glucose turnover may be present in more obese individuals or in those with other comorbid conditions. Insulin secretion and action were significantly reduced in subjects with IGT and diabetes but not in subjects with IFG/NGT compared with those with NFG/NGT. Lack of statistical difference always raises the issue of power. Insulin action as reflected by Si and Si was 20 and 35% lower in subjects with IFG/NGT than in those with NFG/NGT. Assuming the variability in insulin action in these groups would be the same in subsequent studies, we estimate that 143 and 53 additional subjects with IFG/NGT would have to be studied for this difference to become statistically significant. Therefore, if subjects with IFG/NGT have a defect in insulin action, it appears to be subtle. This of course does not exclude the possibility that defects in insulin action and/or insulin secretion subsequently will develop if glucose tolerance deteriorates over time.
In summary, when considered as a group, glucose increased to higher concentrations in individuals with IFG following ingestion of a carbohydrate-containing mixed meal than in individuals with NFG. The excessive rise in glucose was due to lower rates of glucose disposal, since postprandial suppression of EGP and the systemic rate of appearance of the ingested glucose did not differ in individuals with IFG or NFG. Subgroup analysis indicated that postprandial glucose disappearance progressively decreased as glucose tolerance deteriorated, being lowest in individuals with IFG/IGT or IFG/diabetes. Insulin secretion and action were most impaired in these individuals, presumably accounting for the reduction in disposal. Insulin secretion and action also were impaired in individuals with NFG/IGT; however, the defects were less severe and therefore resulted in a smaller reduction in postprandial glucose disposal. On the other hand, insulin secretion, action, and the postprandial pattern of glucose turnover were virtually normal in individuals with isolated IFG, suggesting that the set point but not the subsequent response to glucose was abnormal in these individuals. Thus, it appears that there is substantial heterogeneity in the regulation of postprandial glucose metabolism in individuals with IFG and/or IGT. This implies there are differences in the pathogenesis of pre-diabetes and therefore differences in the risk of subsequently developing diabetes and/or differences in response to therapeutic agents that seek to prevent diabetes.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This study was supported by the U.S. Public Health Service (DK29953 and RR-00585), the Ministero dell’Universita e della Ricerca Scientifica e Tecnologica, Italy, and the Mayo Foundation. G.B. was supported by an American Diabetes Association mentor-based fellowship. G.T. and C.C. were supported by U.S. Public Health Service Grant EB01975.
We thank R. Rood, B. Dicke, and G. DeFoster for technical assistance and assistance in recruiting the subjects; R. Rood for assistance with graphics; M. Aakre and M. Davis for assistance in the preparation of the manuscript; and the staff of the Mayo General Clinical Research Center for assistance in performing the studies.
FOOTNOTES
The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment of page charges. This article must therefore be hereby marked "advertisement" in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.
REFERENCES
Tirosh A, Shai I, Tekes-Manova D, Israeli E, Pereg D, Shochat T, Kochba I, Rudich A, the Israeli Diabetes Research Group: Normal fasting plasma glucose levels and type 2 diabetes in young men. N Engl J Med 353:1454–1462, 2005
Dinneen SF, Maldonado D 3rd, Leibson CL, Klee GG, Li H, Melton LJ 3rd, Rizza RA: Effects of changing diagnostic criteria on the risk of developing diabetes. Diabetes Care 21:1408–1413, 1998
Meigs JB, Muller DC, Nathan DM, Blake DR, Andres R: The natural history of progression from normal glucose tolerance to type 2 diabetes in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Diabetes 52:1475–1484, 2003
Meigs JB, Nathan DM, D’Agostino RB Sr, Wilson PW: Fasting and postchallenge glycemia and cardiovascular disease risk: the Framingham Offspring Study. Diabetes Care 25:1845–1850, 2002
Glucose tolerance and mortality: comparison of WHO and American Diabetes Association diagnostic criteria: the DECODE study group: European Diabetes Epidemiology Group: diabetes epidemiology: collaborative analysis of diagnostic criteria in Europe. Lancet 354:617–621, 1999
Dinneen S, Gerich J, Rizza R: Carbohydrate metabolism in non-insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. N Engl J Med 327:707–713, 1992
Weyer C, Bogardus C, Pratley RE: Metabolic characteristics of individuals with impaired fasting glucose and/or impaired glucose tolerance. Diabetes 48:2197–2203, 1999
Bodkin NL, Metzger BL, Hansen BC: Hepatic glucose production and insulin sensitivity preceding diabetes in monkeys. Am J Physiol 256:E676–E681, 1989
Basu R, Schwenk WF, Rizza RA: Both fasting glucose production and disappearance are abnormal in people with mild and severe type 2 diabetes. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 287:E55–E62, 2004
Rizza RA, Mandarino LJ, Gerich JE: Dose-response characteristics for effects of insulin on production and utilization of glucose in man. Am J Physiol 240:E630–E639, 1981
Vella A, Reed AS, Charkoudian N, Shah P, Basu R, Basu A, Joyner MJ, Rizza RA: Glucose-induced suppression of endogenous glucose production: dynamic response to differing glucose profiles. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 285:E25–E30, 2003
Ader M, Ni TC, Bergman RN: Glucose effectiveness assessed under dynamic and steady state conditions: comparability of uptake versus production components. J Clin Invest 99:1187–1199, 1997
Cherrington AD, Stevenson RW, Steiner KE, Davis MA, Myers SR, Adkins BA, Abumrad NN, Williams PE: Insulin, glucagon, and glucose as regulators of hepatic glucose uptake and production in vivo. Diabetes Metab Rev 3:307–332, 1987
Kahn SE, Prigeon RL, McCulloch DK, Boyko EJ, Bergman RN, Schwartz MW, Neifing JL, Ward WK, Beard JC, Palmer JP, Porte D Jr: Quantification of the relationship between insulin sensitivity and -cell function in human subjects: evidence for a hyperbolic function. Diabetes 42:1663–1672, 1993
Bergman RN, Phillips LS, Cobelli C: Physiologic evaluation of factors controlling glucose tolerance in man: measurement of insulin sensitivity and beta-cell glucose sensitivity from the response to intravenous glucose. J Clin Invest 68:1456–1467, 1981
Sacca L, Orofino G, Petrone A, Vigorito C: Differential roles of splanchnic and peripheral tissues in the pathogenesis of impaired glucose tolerance. J Clin Invest 73:1683–1687, 1984
DeFronzo RA, Ferrannini E, Hendler R, Felig P, Wahren J: Regulation of splanchnic and peripheral glucose uptake by insulin and hyperglycemia in man. Diabetes 32:35–45, 1983
Basu R, Di Camillo B, Toffolo G, Basu A, Shah P, Vella A, Rizza R, Cobelli C: Use of a novel triple-tracer approach to assess postprandial glucose metabolism. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 284:E55–E69, 2003
Breda E, Cavaghan MK, Toffolo G, Polonsky KS, Cobelli C: Oral glucose tolerance test minimal model indexes of -cell function and insulin sensitivity. Diabetes 50:150–158, 2001
Toffolo G, Cefalu WT, Cobelli C: Beta-cell function during insulin-modified intravenous glucose tolerance test successfully assessed by the C-peptide minimal model. Metabolism 48:1162–1166, 1999
Basu R, Breda E, Oberg AL, Powell CC, Dalla Man C, Basu A, Vittone JL, Klee GG, Arora P, Jensen MD, Toffolo G, Cobelli C, Rizza RA: Mechanisms of the age-associated deterioration in glucose tolerance: contribution of alterations in insulin secretion, action, and clearance. Diabetes 52:1738–1748, 2003
Dalla Man C, Yarasheski KE, Caumo A, Robertson H, Toffolo G, Polonsky KS, Cobelli C: Insulin sensitivity by oral glucose minimal models: validation against clamp. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 289:E954–E959, 2005
Dalla Man C, Caumo A, Basu R, Rizza R, Toffolo G, Cobelli C: Measurement of selective effect of insulin on glucose disposal from labeled glucose oral test minimal model. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 289:E909–E914, 2005
Dalla Man C, Caumo A, Basu R, Rizza R, Toffolo G, Cobelli C: Minimal model estimation of glucose absorption and insulin sensitivity from oral test: validation with a tracer method. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 287:E637–E643, 2004
Caumo A, Bergman RN, Cobelli C: Insulin sensitivity from meal tolerance tests in normal subjects: a minimal model index. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 85:4396–4402, 2000
McMahon M, Marsh H, Rizza R: Comparison of the pattern of postprandial carbohydrate metabolism after ingestion of a glucose drink or a mixed meal. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 68:647–653, 1989
Singhal P, Caumo A, Carey PE, Cobelli C, Taylor R: Regulation of endogenous glucose production after a mixed meal in type 2 diabetes. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 283:E275–E283, 2002
Beylot M, Previs SF, David F, Brunengraber H: Determination of the 13C-labeling pattern of glucose by gas chromatography-mass spectrometry. Anal Biochem 212:526–531, 1993
Radziuk J, Norwich KH, Vranic M: Experimental validation of measurements of glucose turnover in nonsteady state. Am J Physiol 234:E84–E93, 1978
Van Cauter E, Mestrez F, Sturis J, Polonsky KS: Estimation of insulin secretion rates from C-peptide levels: comparison of individual and standard kinetic parameters for C-peptide clearance. Diabetes 41:368–377, 1992
Barrett PH, Bell BM, Cobelli C, Golde H, Schumitzky A, Vicini P, Foster DM: SAAM II: simulation, analysis, and modeling software for tracer and pharmacokinetic studies. Metabolism 47:484–492, 1998
Toffolo G, Campioni M, Basu R, Rizza RA, Cobelli C: A minimal model of insulin secretion and kinetics to assess hepatic insulin extraction. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 290:E169–E176, 2006
Lillioja S, Mott DM, Howard BV, Bennett PH, Yki-Jarvinen H, Freymond D, Nyomba BL, Zurlo F, Swinburn B, Bogardus C: Impaired glucose tolerance as a disorder of insulin action: longitudinal and cross-sectional studies in Pima Indians. N Engl J Med 318:1217–1225, 1988
Firth RG, Bell PM, Marsh HM, Hansen I, Rizza RA: Postprandial hyperglycemia in patients with noninsulin-dependent diabetes mellitus: role of hepatic and extrahepatic tissues. J Clin Invest 77:1525–1532, 1986
Ferrannini E, Simonson DC, Katz LD, Reichard G Jr, Bevilacqua S, Barrett EJ, Olsson M, DeFronzo RA: The disposal of an oral glucose load in patients with non-insulin-dependent diabetes. Metabolism 37:79–85, 1988
Kelley D, Mokan M, Veneman T: Impaired postprandial glucose utilization in non-insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. Metabolism 43:1549–1557, 1994
Mitrakou A, Kelley D, Veneman T, Jenssen T, Pangburn T, Reilly J, Gerich J: Contribution of abnormal muscle and liver glucose metabolism to postprandial hyperglycemia in NIDDM. Diabetes 39:1381–1390, 1990
Basu A, Alzaid A, Dinneen S, Caumo A, Cobelli C, Rizza RA: Effects of a change in the pattern of insulin delivery on carbohydrate tolerance in diabetic and nondiabetic humans in the presence of differing degrees of insulin resistance. J Clin Invest 97:2351–2361, 1996
O’Rahilly S, Hattersley A, Vaag A, Gray H: Insulin resistance as the major cause of impaired glucose tolerance: a self-fulfilling prophesy Lancet 344:585–589, 1994
Prigeon RL, Roder ME, Porte D Jr, Kahn SE: The effect of insulin dose on the measurement of insulin sensitivity by the minimal model technique: evidence for saturable insulin transport in humans. J Clin Invest 97:501–507, 1996
Doeden B, Rizza R: Use of a variable insulin infusion to assess insulin action in obesity: defects in both the kinetics and amplitude of response. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 64:902–908, 1987
Bergman RN, Finegood DT, Ader M: Assessment of insulin sensitivity in vivo. Endocr Rev 6:45–86, 1985
Hanefeld M, Koehler C, Fuecker K, Henkel E, Schaper F, Temelkova-Kurktschiev T: Insulin secretion and insulin sensitivity pattern is different in isolated impaired glucose tolerance and impaired fasting glucose: the risk factor in Impaired Glucose Tolerance for Atherosclerosis and Diabetes study. Diabetes Care 26:868–874, 2003
Tripathy D, Carlsson M, Almgren P, Isomaa B, Taskinen MR, Tuomi T, Groop LC: Insulin secretion and insulin sensitivity in relation to glucose tolerance: lessons from the Botnia Study. Diabetes 49:975–980, 2000
Jensen CC, Cnop M, Hull RL, Fujimoto WY, Kahn SE: -Cell function is a major contributor to oral glucose tolerance in high-risk relatives of four ethnic groups in the U.S. Diabetes 51:2170–2178, 2002
Li CL, Tsai ST, Chou P: Relative role of insulin resistance and beta-cell dysfunction in the progression to type 2 diabetes: the Kinmen Study. Diabetes Res Clin Pract 59:225–232, 2003
Kim DJ, Lee MS, Kim KW, Lee MK: Insulin secretory dysfunction and insulin resistance in the pathogenesis of korean type 2 diabetes mellitus. Metabolism 50:590–593, 2001
Carnevale Schianca GP, Rossi A, Sainaghi PP, Maduli E, Bartoli E: The significance of impaired fasting glucose versus impaired glucose tolerance: importance of insulin secretion and resistance. Diabetes Care 26:1333–1337, 2003
Utzschneider KM, Prigeon RL, Carr DB, Hull RL, Tong J, Shofer JB, Retzlaff BM, Knopp RH, Kahn SE: Impact of differences in fasting glucose and glucose tolerance on the hyperbolic relationship between insulin sensitivity and insulin responses. Diabetes Care 29:356–362, 2006
Ferrannini E, Gastaldelli A, Miyazaki Y, Matsuda M, Mari A, DeFronzo RA: Beta-cell function in subjects spanning the range from normal glucose tolerance to overt diabetes: a new analysis. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 90:493–500, 2005
Rutter GA: Visualising insulin secretion: the Minkowski Lecture 2004. Diabetologia 47:1861–1872, 2004
Barg S, Eliasson L, Renstrom E, Rorsman P: A subset of 50 secretory granules in close contact with L-type Ca2+ channels accounts for first-phase insulin secretion in mouse -cells. Diabetes 51 (Suppl. 1):S74–S82, 2002
Duncan RR, Greaves J, Wiegand UK, Matskevich I, Bodammer G, Apps DK, Shipston MJ, Chow RH: Functional and spatial segregation of secretory vesicle pools according to vesicle age. Nature 422:176–180, 2003
Stumvoll M, Tataranni PA, Stefan N, Vozarova B, Bogardus C: Glucose allostasis. Diabetes 52:903–909, 2003(Gerlies Bock, Chiara Dalla Man, Marco Ca)
2 Department of Electronics and Informatics, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
DI, disposition index; EGP, endogenous glucose production; IFG, impaired fasting glucose; IGT, impaired glucose tolerance; NFG, normal fasting glucose; NGT, normal glucose tolerance; OGTT, oral glucose tolerance test
ABSTRACT
Thirty-two subjects with impaired fasting glucose (IFG) and 28 subjects with normal fasting glucose (NFG) ingested a labeled meal and 75 g glucose (oral glucose tolerance test) on separate occasions. Fasting glucose, insulin, and C-peptide were higher (P < 0.05) in subjects with IFG than in those with NFG, whereas endogenous glucose production (EGP) did not differ, indicating hepatic insulin resistance. EGP was promptly suppressed, and meal glucose appearance comparably increased following meal ingestion in both groups. In contrast, glucose disappearance (Rd) immediately after meal ingestion was lower (P < 0.001) in subjects with IFG/impaired glucose tolerance (IGT) and IFG/diabetes but did not differ in subjects with IFG/normal glucose tolerance (NGT) or NFG/NGT. Net insulin action (Si) and insulin-stimulated glucose disposal (Si) were reduced (P < 0.001, ANOVA) in subjects with NFG/IGT, IFG/IGT, and IFG/diabetes but did not differ in subjects with NFG/NGT or IFG/NGT. Defective insulin secretion also contributed to lower postprandial Rd since disposition indexes were lower (P < 0.001, ANOVA) in subjects with NFG/IGT, IFG/IGT, and IFG/diabetes but did not differ in subjects with NFG/NGT and IFG/NGT. We conclude that postprandial hyperglycemia in individuals with early diabetes is due to lower rates of glucose disappearance rather than increased meal appearance or impaired suppression of EGP, regardless of their fasting glucose. In contrast, insulin secretion, action, and the pattern of postprandial turnover are essentially normal in individuals with isolated IFG.
Individuals with impaired fasting glucose (IFG) have a 20–30% chance of developing diabetes over the next 5–10 years (1–3). The risk is even greater if they have combined IFG and impaired glucose tolerance (IGT). Furthermore, IFG and IGT are associated with increased risk of cardiovascular events (4,5). Therefore, the pathogenesis of IFG alone or in combination with IGT has engendered considerable interest. Glucose concentration begins to increase when glucose appearance exceeds glucose disappearance and continues to increase until these two rates are once again equal. In the fasting state, glucose appearance is determined by the rate of glucose release from the liver with perhaps a small contribution by the kidney. Together, these processes are referred to as endogenous glucose production (EGP). The situation is more complex following food ingestion when glucose appearance equals the sum of EGP and the rate of appearance of the ingested glucose (6). There is currently limited data as to the contribution of these processes to fasting hyperglycemia (7,8) and no data regarding the regulation of postprandial glucose metabolism in individuals with IFG. The latter is of particular interest since while most individuals with IFG also have either IGT or diabetes, some have normal glucose tolerance (NGT). On the other hand, some individuals with normal fasting glucose (NFG) have IGT.
When considered in the light of the prevailing glucose and insulin concentration, EGP is increased in individuals with mild and severe type 2 diabetes (9). To our knowledge, Weyer et al. (7) are the only investigators who have measured EGP in individuals with IFG. In those studies, fasting EGP was increased in Pima Indians with IFG, regardless of whether they had NGT or IGT. In contrast, fasting glucose production was not elevated in Pima Indians with IGT and NFG concentrations. However, since fasting insulin concentrations were elevated in those subjects, this implies the presence of hepatic insulin resistance. On the other hand, since glucose and insulin suppress glucose production (10–13) and enhanced insulin secretion can potentially compensate for a defect in insulin action (14,15), hepatic insulin resistance does not necessarily mean that excessive EGP is the cause of postprandial hyperglycemia in individuals with IFG and/or IGT. Conversely, effective compensation via these mechanisms could normalize postprandial suppression of EGP, thereby enabling some individuals with IFG to maintain normal postprandial glucose concentrations.
To our knowledge, the postprandial rate of meal glucose appearance has not been measured in individuals with IFG. However, Sacca et al. (16) reported that splanchnic glucose uptake during intravenous glucose infusion was greater in six subjects with combined IFG/IGT than in seven normoglycemic control subjects, implying enhanced rather than decreased hepatic glucose extraction. Unfortunately, these data are difficult to interpret since glucose concentrations were far higher in subjects with IFG than in control subjects, and glucose is a potent stimulus of hepatic glucose uptake (13,17). Therefore, the relative contribution of alterations in meal-derived glucose appearance, EGP, or glucose disappearance to the regulation of postprandial glucose concentrations in individuals with IFG is presently not known.
To address these questions, meal glucose appearance, EGP, and glucose disappearance were measured in 32 subjects with IFG before and after ingestion of a mixed meal using a validated triple-tracer approach (18). Insulin secretion and action were concurrently measured using the C-peptide (19,20) and "oral" minimal models (21–25) in order to gain insight regarding the etiology of alterations (if observed) in postprandial glucose metabolism. Results were compared with those observed in 28 control subjects with NFG. In addition, to gain insight regarding the heterogeneity of glucose tolerance in individuals with NFG or IFG, the subjects were subdivided according to whether they also fulfilled oral glucose tolerance test (OGTT) criteria for IGT or diabetes.
RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODS
After approval from the Mayo Institutional Review Board, 32 subjects (17 women and 15 men) with IFG and 28 subjects (17 women and 11 men) with NFG gave informed written consent to participate in the study. All subjects were Caucasian, in good health, at a stable weight, and did not engage in regular vigorous physical exercise. At the time of study, subjects were on no medications other than a stable dose of thyroid hormone, low-dose aspirin, hydroxymethylglutaryl-CoA reductase inhibitors, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor antidepressants, or antihypertensives, which are metabolically neutral (e.g., no ACE inhibitors or -blockers).
All subjects were instructed to follow a weight-maintenance diet containing 55% carbohydrate, 30% fat, and 15% protein for at least 3 days before the study date. Fasting plasma glucose concentration was measured after an overnight fast on two separate occasions at least 1 week apart. Subjects whose average fasting glucose level was <5.2 mmol/l or between 5.6 and 7.0 mmol/l were selected for the study and referred to as having NFG or IFG, respectively. Subjects with a fasting glucose between 5.2 and 5.6 mmol/l were excluded from the study, since despite having glucose concentrations within the normal range, previous studies have shown that such individuals have an 8% risk of developing diabetes within the next 10 years and, therefore, possibly represent an early form of IFG (1–3). Eligible subjects were then admitted to the Mayo General Clinical Research Center on two subsequent occasions at 1700 the evening before the study and ate a standard 10 kcal/kg meal (55% carbohydrate, 30% fat, and 15% protein) between 1830 and 1900. No additional food was eaten until the next morning. On one occasion, subjects ingested 75 g glucose after a 12-h overnight fast. Based on these results, subjects with either NFG or IFG were subclassified as having NFG/NGT (2-h plasma glucose <7.8 mmol/l), NFG/IGT (2-h plasma glucose between 7.8 and 11.1 mmol/l), IFG/IGT, or IFG/diabetes (2-h plasma glucose >11.1 mmol/l).
On another occasion, subjects ingested a labeled mixed meal as previously described (18). In brief, an 18-gauge cannula was inserted at 0600 into a forearm vein for tracer infusions. Another 18-gauge cannula was inserted in a retrograde fashion in a dorsal hand vein of the opposite arm, and the hand was placed in a heated box (55°C) to enable sampling of arterialized venous blood. A primed (12 mg/kg) continuous (0.12 mg · kg–1 · min–1) infusion of [6,6-2H2]glucose (MassTrace, Woburn, MA) started at 0700. At time 0, i.e., 1000, subjects ingested a standard mixed meal within 15 min, which consisted of three scrambled eggs, 55 g Canadian bacon (or 47 g steak), and Jell-O containing 75 g glucose that was enriched (to 4%) with [1-13C]glucose, as previously described (18,26). An intravenous infusion of [6-3H]glucose was started at the same time and infused in a pattern that minimized the change in the ratio of plasma tracer (i.e., [6-3H]glucose) to plasma meal tracee (i.e., [1-13C]glucose). In addition, the [6,6-2H2]glucose infusion was varied in a manner mimicking the anticipated pattern of change of EGP, thereby also minimizing the change in the ratio of plasma tracer (i.e., [6,6-2H2]glucose) to plasma tracee (i.e., concentration of endogenous plasma glucose), as previously described (18,27).
Analytical techniques.
Plasma samples were placed on ice, centrifuged at 4°C, separated, and stored at –20°C until the assay was complete. Glucose concentrations were measured using a glucose oxidase method (Yellow Springs Instruments, Yellow Springs, OH). Plasma insulin was measured using a chemiluminescence assay with reagents obtained from Beckman (Access Assay; Beckman, Chaska, MN). Plasma glucagon and C-peptide were measured by radioimmunoassay using reagents supplied by Linco Research (St. Louis, MO). Body composition was measured using dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry (DPX Scanner; Lunar, Madison, WI) and computerized absorption tomography with cuts at L2/3 and T11/12 to determine percent body fat and visceral fat. Plasma [6,6-2H2]glucose and [1-13C]glucose enrichments were measured using gas chromatography–mass spectrometry (Thermoquest, San Jose, CA) to simultaneously monitor the C-1, C-2, and C-3 to C-6 fragments, as described by Beylot et al. (28), and [6-3H]glucose specific activity by liquid-scintillation counting following deproteinization and passage over anion and cation exchange columns.
Calculations.
The systemic rates of meal appearance (RaMEAL), EGP, and glucose disappearance (Rd) were calculated using Radziuk’s two-compartment model (29), as previously described (18). In brief, RaMEAL was calculated by multiplying the rate of appearance of [1-13C]glucose (obtained from the infusion rate of [6-3H]glucose and the clamped plasma ratio of [6-3H]glucose and [1-13C]glucose) by the meal enrichment (i.e., the ratio of total glucose to tracer in the meal). EGP was calculated from the infusion rate of [6,62H2]glucose and the clamped plasma ratio of [6,62H2]glucose to endogenous glucose concentration. Glucose disappearance was calculated by subtracting the change in glucose mass from the overall rate of glucose appearance (i.e., RaMEAL + EGP).
Insulin sensitivity (Si), which measures the overall effect of insulin to stimulate glucose disposal and inhibit glucose production, was estimated from plasma glucose and insulin concentrations using the oral glucose minimal model (24,25). The model assumes that insulin action on glucose production and disposal emanates from a compartment remote from plasma, which is usually identified with the interstitium. Similarly, the selective effect of insulin on glucose disposal (Si) was estimated from oral-ingested glucose tracer and insulin concentration by using the labeled oral glucose minimal model (23).
-Cell responsivity indexes were estimated from plasma glucose and C-peptide concentrations measured during the test by using the oral C-peptide minimal model (19), incorporating age-associated changes in C-peptide kinetics as measured by Van Cauter et al. (30). The model assumes that insulin secretion is made up of two components. The dynamic component is likely to represent secretion of promptly releasable insulin and is proportional to the rate of increase of glucose concentration through a parameter, Phidynamic, which defines the dynamic responsivity index. The static component is derived from the provision of new insulin to the releasable pool and is characterized by a static index and by a delay time constant, T. The meaning of Phistatic and T can be made clear with reference to a response to an above-basal step increase of glucose; provision tends, with time constant T, toward a steady state, which is linearly related to the glucose step size through parameter Phistatic. To determine if insulin secretion indexes were appropriate for the prevailing level of insulin action, the disposition indexes (DIs) DItotal, DIdynamic, and DIstatic were calculated by multiplying Phitotal, Phidynamic, and Phistatic, respectively, by Si.
Values from –30 to 0 min were averaged and considered as basal. The area above basal was calculated using the trapezoidal rule. Parameters of all models were estimated by using the SAAMII software (31). Measurement errors were assumed to be independent and Gaussian, with zero mean and variance for glucose and tracer glucose (22) and for C-peptide (32).
Statistical analysis.
All data are presented as means ± SE. Rates of glucose turnover are expressed as micromoles per kilogram lean body mass. Two sample comparisons between subjects with IFG and NFG were made using Student’s t test or rank-sum test for data that were nonnormally distributed. Analyses among the NFG and IFG subgroups were made using ANOVA followed, where appropriate, by Student’s two-tailed nonpaired t test. A P value <0.05 was considered statistically significant.
RESULTS
By design, the fasting plasma glucose levels at screening were higher (P < 0.001) in subjects with IFG than in those with NFG/NGT (Table 1). Age, lean body mass, and body fat did not differ statistically between groups. On the other hand, BMI and visceral fat were greater (P < 0.05) in subjects with versus NFG/NGT. None of the above parameters differed in subjects with IFG with or without IGT or diabetes or in subjects with NFG with or without IGT. Sixty percent of the subjects with IFG/IGT and 50% of subjects with IFG/diabetes had a history of diabetes in a first-degree relative. No subject with IFG/NGT had a family history of diabetes. By selection, none of the subjects with NFG had a family history of diabetes.
Plasma glucose, insulin, C-peptide, and glucagon concentrations.
Fasting plasma glucose concentrations were higher (P < 0.001) in the total group with IFG than in subjects with NFG/NGT (Fig. 1) and increased to a higher peak (P < 0.002) following meal ingestion (11.9 ± 0.4 vs. 9.9 ± 0.2 mmol/l). On the other hand, the glucose area above basal did not differ statistically between these two groups. When analyzed according to OGTT status, fasting plasma glucose concentrations were higher (P < 0.001) in subjects with IFG/diabetes but did not differ in the those with IFG/NGT and IFG/IGT (Fig. 2). Fasting glucose also did not differ in the subjects with NFG/IGT and NFG/NGT. Following meal ingestion, glucose concentration increased to a higher (P < 0.001) peak in the subjects with IFG/IGT (11.2 ± 0.4 mmol/l) and IFG/diabetes (14.5 ± 0.4 mmol/l) compared with subjects with NFG/NGT. On the other hand, peak glucose concentration did not differ in subjects with IFG/NGT and NFG/NGT (10.6 ± 0.6 vs. 9.9 ± 0.2 mmol/l, respectively), resulting in a lower (P < 0.05) postprandial increment in glucose in subjects with IFG/NGT. Peak glucose concentrations also did not differ in the subjects with NFG/IGT and NFG/NGT (10.4 ± 0.4 vs. 9.9 ± 0.2 mmol/l, respectively); however, it took longer for glucose concentrations to return to preprandial levels in the former, resulting in a greater (P < 0.05) glucose area above basal in the subjects with NFG/IGT. Glucose area above basal was also greater (P < 0.001) in the subjects with IFG/diabetes than in those with NFG/NGT.
Fasting plasma insulin concentrations were higher (P < 0.05) in the total group with IFG than in the subjects with NFG/NGT (Fig. 1) but rose to a comparable peak after meal ingestion (628 ± 45 vs. 531 ± 86 pmol/l, respectively) (Table 2). However, the time to peak was longer (P < 0.01) in the total group with IFG than in the subjects with NFG/NGT (88 ± 7 vs. 57 ± 6 min, respectively). Subgroups analysis (Fig. 2) indicated that the higher fasting plasma insulin concentrations in the total group with IFG were primarily due to higher insulin concentrations in subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes (P < 0.05 vs. NFG/NGT) since insulin concentrations did not differ among the other groups. The insulin area above basal did not differ among the groups during the 1st hour after meal ingestion (i.e., when glucose concentrations were diverging in the subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes). The insulin area above basal during the entire 6-h observation also did not differ among the subjects with NGT and IGT but was higher (P < 0.05) in the subjects with IFG/diabetes than in those with NFG/NGT. However, the time to peak insulin concentration was longer (P < 0.01) in the subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes (92 ± 8 and 115 ± 11 min, respectively) than in those with NFG/NGT.
Fasting plasma C-peptide concentrations also were higher (P < 0.05) in the total group with IFG than in the subjects with NFG/NGT and rose to a comparable peak after meal ingestion (3.4 ± 0.2 vs. 3.0 ± 0.3 nmol/l, respectively). As with insulin, the higher fasting C-peptide concentrations in the total group with IFG were primarily due to higher C-peptide concentrations in subjects with IFG/diabetes (P < 0.001 vs. NFG/NGT). On the other hand, the C-peptide area above basal during the first 60 min after meal ingestion, and during the entire 6 h of observation, did not differ among subjects with NGT and IGT. In contrast, the C-peptide area above basal over the 6 h of observation was higher (P < 0.05) in subjects with IFG/diabetes than in those with NFG/NGT.
Fasting plasma glucagon concentrations were higher (P < 0.02) in the total group with IFG than in the subjects with NFG/NGT before meal ingestion (75 ± 4 vs. 57 ± 4 pg/ml, respectively) but did not differ after meal ingestion. While fasting plasma glucagon concentrations were somewhat higher in the subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes compared with those with NFG/NGT, the differences were not significant. Plasma glucagon concentrations remained constant or tended to fall immediately after meal ingestion in all subgroups, then subsequently rose as glucose concentrations fell back toward preprandial values.
Meal glucose appearance, EGP, and glucose disappearance.
The systemic rate of appearance of meal-derived glucose reached a comparable peak within 30–60 min after meal ingestion in subjects with IFG and NFG/NGT. While the area above basal did not differ among groups, the area above baseline over the 6 h of observation tended (P = 0.08) to be lower in the subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes compared with the subjects with NFG/NGT.
Despite increased fasting glucose and insulin concentrations, fasting EGP did not differ in subjects with IFG and NFG. Subgroup analysis indicated that EGP also did not differ in subjects with or without IGT and was slightly higher (P = 0.08) in subjects with IFG/diabetes. In addition, postprandial suppression of endogenous glucose (area below basal) following meal ingestion also did not differ between groups.
Glucose disappearance before meal ingestion also did not differ in the total goup with IFG and the subjects with NFG/NGT or in the various subgroups. On the other hand, the increment above basal during the 1st hour after meal ingestion was smaller (P < 0.002) in the total group with IFG than in the subjects with NFG/NGT (Fig. 3). The blunted increase in glucose disappearance immediately after meal ingestion was primarily due to a smaller increase in the subjects with IFG/diabetes (P < 0.001) and IFG/IGT (P < 0.001) (Fig. 4). The increase in glucose disappearance during the 1st hour after meal ingestion tended to be lower (P = 0.08) in the subjects with NFG/IGT than in those with NFG/NGT. Glucose disappearance reached comparable rates in all groups by 2 h after meal ingestion and was slightly higher in the subjects with IGT and diabetes than in those with NGT, resulting in a comparable total area above basal during the 6 h of observation.
Indexes of insulin action.
Net insulin action (Si) and the ability of insulin to stimulate glucose uptake (Si) were measured following meal ingestion with the respective unlabeled and labeled oral minimal models (Fig. 5). Si (P < 0.005) and Si (P < 0.001) were lower in the total group with IFG than in the subjects with NFG/NGT. Subgroup analysis indicated that Si and Si were lower (P < 0.005) in subjects with NFG/IGT, IFG/IGT, and IFG/diabetes than in those with NFG/NGT. Si and Si were also lower (P < 0.05) in the subjects with IFG/diabetes than in those with IFG/NGT. On the other hand, while Si and Si were numerically lower in the subjects with IFG/NGT versus those with NFG/NGT, the differences were not significant.
Indexes of insulin secretion.
Indexes of insulin secretion were measured following meal ingestion with the C-peptide minimal model, which enabled concurrent assessment with insulin action (Fig. 6). The overall response to glucose (Phitotal), the response to a change in glucose (Phidynamic), and the response to a given glucose level (Phistatic) did not differ in the total group with IFG and those with NFG/NGT. However, when DIs were calculated to determine if insulin secretion was appropriate for the prevailing level of insulin action, DItotal (P < 0.01), DIstatic (P = 0.01), and DIdynamic (P < 0.001) were all lower in the total group with IFG than in those with NFG/NGT.
Phidynamic did not differ among the subjects with NFG/NGT, IFG/NGT, and NFG/IGT but tended to be lower in those with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes. Phitotal and Phistatic also did not differ among the groups. However, the time required to reach Phistatic (37 ± 5 vs. 14 ± 2 min) was longer (P < 0.001) in those with IFG/diabetes than in those with NFG/NGT but did not differ among the other groups. On the other hand, when the appropriateness of insulin secretion for the prevailing level of insulin action was considered, DItotal, DIdynamic, and DIstaticall were lower in subjects with NFG/IGT (P < 0.05), IFG/IGT (P < 0.01), and IFG/diabetes (P < 0.001) than in those with NFG/NGT. Furthermore, DItotal and DIstatic were also lower in subjects with NFG/IGT (P < 0.01), IFG/IGT (P < 0.005), and IFG/diabetes (P < 0.001) compared with those with IFG/NGT, and DIdynamic was lower (P < 0.01) in subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes than in those with IFG/NGT. While DItotal, DIdynamic, and DIstatic did not differ in subjects with NFG/IGT and IFG/IGT, they tended to be lower (P = 0.06) in subjects with IFG/diabetes compared with those with NFG/IGT. On the other hand, none of the DIs differed in subjects with IFG/NFG and NFG/NGT.
DISCUSSION
People with pre-diabetes (i.e., IFG and/or IGT) are at increased risk of developing overt diabetes (1–3). The present study indicates that fasting EGP is inappropriately increased and glucose disappearance is inappropriately decreased in individuals with IFG when considered in light of the higher prevailing glucose and insulin concentrations. It therefore appears that both abnormalities contribute to IFG. On the other hand, EGP is promptly suppressed in individuals with IFG and/or IGT after meal ingestion. The rate of meal appearance also did not differ among groups, indicating that excessive glucose absorption and/or decreased hepatic glucose uptake does not cause postprandial hyperglycemia in individuals with pre-diabetes. In contrast, postprandial glucose disposal was decreased in individuals with IGT or diabetes, with the decrease being most evident in individuals who also had IFG. Both the defects in insulin secretion and the ability of insulin to stimulate glucose uptake contributed to the lower rates of disposal, since both were impaired in individuals with IGT or diabetes. On the other hand, postprandial EGP, meal appearance, glucose disposal, insulin secretion, and insulin action were all normal in individuals with isolated IFG, implying a set point abnormality with an intact -cell response to food ingestion. Taken together, these data indicate that both the defects in insulin secretion and action contribute to postprandial hyperglycemia. They also indicate that the pattern of postprandial glucose metabolism is essentially normal in individuals with isolated IFG, perhaps presaging a lower risk of progression to overt diabetes.
Rates and disappearance of EGP did not differ in subjects with IFG and NFG before meal ingestion. However, since both glucose and insulin concentrations were higher in the former than in the latter, fasting EGP was inappropriately elevated and disappearance inappropriately reduced. Subgroup analysis indicated that the increase in fasting glucose production was most evident in subjects with the worst glucose tolerance (i.e., subjects with IFG/diabetes). This observation supports prior reports by Weyer et al. (7) and Lillioja et al. (33) that Pima Indians with IFG have both hepatic and extrahepatic insulin resistance. Of interest, fasting glucagon concentrations were higher in subjects with IFG than in subjects with NFG/NGT. The increase in fasting glucagon appeared to be most evident in the subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes; however, the number of subjects in these subgroups was relatively small, and the differences were not significant. Nevertheless, these data suggest that glucagon may contribute to increased fasting glucose concentrations in individuals with IFG.
EGP rapidly suppressed in all groups, following meal ingestion, presumably reflecting the combined suppressive effects of the concurrent increases in glucose and insulin (9–13). While the rate of suppression during the 1st hour after food ingestion tended to be slower in subjects with IFG/diabetes, it did not differ significantly from that observed in subjects with NFG. This pattern is not dissimilar to the situation observed in individuals with overt diabetes who also ultimately suppress EGP to normal following food ingestion (34–37). However, since the preprandial rates are increased and defects in insulin secretion are more marked in individuals with "severe" diabetes, the time to suppression is delayed and excess amounts of glucose enter circulation. Taken together, these data lend additional support to the concept that hepatic insulin resistance occurs early in the evolution of type 2 diabetes. On the other hand, failure to appropriately suppress EGP is not the cause of postprandial hyperglycemia in individuals with IGT or early diabetes.
The systemic rate of appearance of the ingested glucose did not differ in subjects with IFG and NFG and, if anything, tended to be lower in the subgroups with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes, perhaps reflecting increased hepatic glucose uptake due to higher portal glucose concentrations. Therefore, increased meal glucose appearance was not the cause of the higher postprandial glucose concentrations in any of the groups. In contrast, postprandial glucose disappearance was lower in subjects with IFG immediately after meal ingestion, particularly during the 1st hour when the excessive rise in glucose occurred in subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes. Of note, glucose disappearance in subjects with NFG/IGT during the 1st hour after meal ingestion only slightly reduced, perhaps accounting for the fact that the peak glucose concentration in this group did not differ from that in subjects with NFG/NGT. However, it took longer for glucose to return to preprandial concentrations, resulting in higher glucose concentrations at 2 h in subjects with NFG/IGT. Since insulin secretion was relatively intact in these individuals, whereas insulin action was markedly decreased, this postprandial pattern of change in glucose concentrations (and the OGTT pattern, since that is why subjects in this group were classified as having IGT) is consistent with previous reports indicating that a delay and decrease in early insulin secretion results in a higher peak glucose concentration, whereas a defect in insulin action results in an increased duration of hyperglycemia (38,39). Of interest, subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes had both a higher peak glucose concentration and a more prolonged duration of hyperglycemia. This occurred since they were both insulin resistant and had a severe defect in their ability to secrete insulin in response to the rapid increase in glucose, which occurred immediately after meal ingestion.
Combined use of the oral minimal and C-peptide models enabled indexes of insulin action and insulin secretion to be simultaneously measured under physiologic conditions in the same individual. This is important since insulin action is context dependent in that it differs depending on how much insulin is given and the pattern in which it is given (40–42). Therefore, the observation that net insulin action (Si) was decreased in subjects with IFG following meal ingestion is reassuring and consistent with previous studies that have measured net insulin action in the fasting state using the homeostasis model assessment method (43–48), following intravenous glucose injection or during insulin infusion (7,49). The labeled oral minimal model established that the decrease in net insulin action was due, at least in part, to a decrease in the ability of insulin to stimulate glucose uptake (Si). Subgroup analysis indicated that this was primarily due to reduced insulin action in subjects with IGT or diabetes, irrespective of their fasting glucose concentration. On the other hand, insulin action did not differ significantly in subjects with NFG/NGT and IFG/NGT.
DIdynamic, which assesses the appropriateness of insulin secretion in response to a change in glucose, was impaired in all of the subjects with IGT, with the severity of the defect increasing as glucose tolerance deteriorated. To our knowledge, the only other study examining a similar aspects of insulin secretion is that of Ferrannini et al. (50). The authors used a model to evaluate insulin secretion in individuals with IGT during an OGTT, which was similar to the model used in the present experiments. They reported that while the static response to insulin was decreased in their subjects with IGT, the dynamic response to glucose was intact. However, this discrepancy is likely more apparent than real since the appropriateness of the dynamic response was not considered in light of the 50% reduction in insulin action in subjects with IGT. Furthermore, their IGT group contained subjects with both NFG and IFG. As is evident in the present studies, the impairment in DIdynamic is more marked in individuals with both IFG and IGT than in those with IGT alone, which likely accounts for the higher peak postprandial glucose concentrations in the former than in the latter (see Fig. 2).
The static response was decreased in all subjects with IGT. This observation is consistent with the previous report of Ferraninni et al. (50), which showed that the static response to glucose is decreased during an OGTT in individuals with IGT. The static response to glucose evaluates the amount of insulin that is secreted at any given level of glucose and therefore has been referred to as "glucose sensitivity." Since this response occurs throughout the entire 6 h of study, it presumably is influenced by insulin synthesis and processing, as well as more distal steps in the insulin secretion pathway. The model used in the current experiments indicates that there was a delay between the time when glucose reaches a given level and when the static response achieves a steady state. This time (t in the model) averaged 10 min in subjects with NFG/NGT and tended to be slightly increased (15 min) in subjects with IGT. In contrast, it was markedly prolonged in subjects with IFG/diabetes, averaging 35 min. Of interest, in vitro studies suggest that 8–10 min are required for insulin granules in the storage pool to move to the plasma membrane, dock, and become primed for exocytosis (50–53). It is interesting to speculate that the activation of this process is required for the static response to glucose to achieve a steady state and that the development of diabetes either is exacerbated by or causes a delay in the rate at which insulin granules in the storage pool are primed for secretion.
The results of subjects with IFG/NGT are particularly intriguing. Despite fasting hyperglycemia, postprandial glucose concentrations were virtually identical to those in subjects with NFG/NGT. Since preprandial glucose concentrations were higher, the postprandial increment in glucose concentration was lower than in subjects with NFG/NGT (Fig. 2 and Table 2). This was not due to reduced glucose absorption or increased hepatic glucose uptake, since the rate of appearance of ingested glucose was virtually identical in both groups, as were postprandial changes in EGP and disposal. Insulin action was slightly, but not significantly, lower in subjects with IFG/NGT compared with those with NFG/NGT, perhaps because of greater visceral adiposity in the former. Indexes of insulin secretion including the dynamic and static responses to glucose were normal whether assessed as actual responses (Fig. 6) or when corrected for the degree of insulin resistance by calculating DIs (Fig. 7). Preprandial rates of EGP and disappearance did not differ in subjects with IFG/NGT and NFG/NGT. However, in contrast to subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes, fasting insulin concentrations were not increased, making it difficult to invoke insulin resistance as the cause of fasting hyperglycemia. On the other hand, since glucose production and disappearance did not differ in subjects with IFG/NGT and NFG/NGT and since, as noted above, even small increases in glucose concentrations result in suppression of glucose production (14,15), this implies that glucose concentration continued to increase until what has recently been referred to as "allostasis" (54) was again achieved at a higher glucose concentration.
The observations that fasting insulin concentration was normal, despite a higher fasting glucose concentration, and that insulin secretion in response to a meal-related rise in glucose concentration was also normal argue for a higher "set point" in the subjects with IFG/NGT. A decrease in -cell glucokinase activity in subjects with IFG/NGT could provide an explanation for this pattern of response (39). However, none of subjects with IFG/NGT had a family history of diabetes, which is in contrast to the subjects with IFG/IGT and IFG/diabetes, whereas 50–60% of the subjects had a first-degree relative with diabetes. Since subjects with a defect in glucokinase activity generally have a family history of diabetes, the lack of family history in the subjects with IFG/NGT reduces the likelihood of such a defect. Nevertheless, the cause and potential consequences (e.g., relative risk of subsequently developing diabetes) of elevated fasting but normal postprandial glucose concentrations in these individuals clearly warrant further study.
The present study has certain limitations. The subjects were healthy, had no history of vascular disease, and were only modestly obese (BMI 31 kg/m2). More marked abnormalities in insulin secretion, action, and postprandial glucose turnover may be present in more obese individuals or in those with other comorbid conditions. Insulin secretion and action were significantly reduced in subjects with IGT and diabetes but not in subjects with IFG/NGT compared with those with NFG/NGT. Lack of statistical difference always raises the issue of power. Insulin action as reflected by Si and Si was 20 and 35% lower in subjects with IFG/NGT than in those with NFG/NGT. Assuming the variability in insulin action in these groups would be the same in subsequent studies, we estimate that 143 and 53 additional subjects with IFG/NGT would have to be studied for this difference to become statistically significant. Therefore, if subjects with IFG/NGT have a defect in insulin action, it appears to be subtle. This of course does not exclude the possibility that defects in insulin action and/or insulin secretion subsequently will develop if glucose tolerance deteriorates over time.
In summary, when considered as a group, glucose increased to higher concentrations in individuals with IFG following ingestion of a carbohydrate-containing mixed meal than in individuals with NFG. The excessive rise in glucose was due to lower rates of glucose disposal, since postprandial suppression of EGP and the systemic rate of appearance of the ingested glucose did not differ in individuals with IFG or NFG. Subgroup analysis indicated that postprandial glucose disappearance progressively decreased as glucose tolerance deteriorated, being lowest in individuals with IFG/IGT or IFG/diabetes. Insulin secretion and action were most impaired in these individuals, presumably accounting for the reduction in disposal. Insulin secretion and action also were impaired in individuals with NFG/IGT; however, the defects were less severe and therefore resulted in a smaller reduction in postprandial glucose disposal. On the other hand, insulin secretion, action, and the postprandial pattern of glucose turnover were virtually normal in individuals with isolated IFG, suggesting that the set point but not the subsequent response to glucose was abnormal in these individuals. Thus, it appears that there is substantial heterogeneity in the regulation of postprandial glucose metabolism in individuals with IFG and/or IGT. This implies there are differences in the pathogenesis of pre-diabetes and therefore differences in the risk of subsequently developing diabetes and/or differences in response to therapeutic agents that seek to prevent diabetes.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This study was supported by the U.S. Public Health Service (DK29953 and RR-00585), the Ministero dell’Universita e della Ricerca Scientifica e Tecnologica, Italy, and the Mayo Foundation. G.B. was supported by an American Diabetes Association mentor-based fellowship. G.T. and C.C. were supported by U.S. Public Health Service Grant EB01975.
We thank R. Rood, B. Dicke, and G. DeFoster for technical assistance and assistance in recruiting the subjects; R. Rood for assistance with graphics; M. Aakre and M. Davis for assistance in the preparation of the manuscript; and the staff of the Mayo General Clinical Research Center for assistance in performing the studies.
FOOTNOTES
The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment of page charges. This article must therefore be hereby marked "advertisement" in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.
REFERENCES
Tirosh A, Shai I, Tekes-Manova D, Israeli E, Pereg D, Shochat T, Kochba I, Rudich A, the Israeli Diabetes Research Group: Normal fasting plasma glucose levels and type 2 diabetes in young men. N Engl J Med 353:1454–1462, 2005
Dinneen SF, Maldonado D 3rd, Leibson CL, Klee GG, Li H, Melton LJ 3rd, Rizza RA: Effects of changing diagnostic criteria on the risk of developing diabetes. Diabetes Care 21:1408–1413, 1998
Meigs JB, Muller DC, Nathan DM, Blake DR, Andres R: The natural history of progression from normal glucose tolerance to type 2 diabetes in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Diabetes 52:1475–1484, 2003
Meigs JB, Nathan DM, D’Agostino RB Sr, Wilson PW: Fasting and postchallenge glycemia and cardiovascular disease risk: the Framingham Offspring Study. Diabetes Care 25:1845–1850, 2002
Glucose tolerance and mortality: comparison of WHO and American Diabetes Association diagnostic criteria: the DECODE study group: European Diabetes Epidemiology Group: diabetes epidemiology: collaborative analysis of diagnostic criteria in Europe. Lancet 354:617–621, 1999
Dinneen S, Gerich J, Rizza R: Carbohydrate metabolism in non-insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. N Engl J Med 327:707–713, 1992
Weyer C, Bogardus C, Pratley RE: Metabolic characteristics of individuals with impaired fasting glucose and/or impaired glucose tolerance. Diabetes 48:2197–2203, 1999
Bodkin NL, Metzger BL, Hansen BC: Hepatic glucose production and insulin sensitivity preceding diabetes in monkeys. Am J Physiol 256:E676–E681, 1989
Basu R, Schwenk WF, Rizza RA: Both fasting glucose production and disappearance are abnormal in people with mild and severe type 2 diabetes. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 287:E55–E62, 2004
Rizza RA, Mandarino LJ, Gerich JE: Dose-response characteristics for effects of insulin on production and utilization of glucose in man. Am J Physiol 240:E630–E639, 1981
Vella A, Reed AS, Charkoudian N, Shah P, Basu R, Basu A, Joyner MJ, Rizza RA: Glucose-induced suppression of endogenous glucose production: dynamic response to differing glucose profiles. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 285:E25–E30, 2003
Ader M, Ni TC, Bergman RN: Glucose effectiveness assessed under dynamic and steady state conditions: comparability of uptake versus production components. J Clin Invest 99:1187–1199, 1997
Cherrington AD, Stevenson RW, Steiner KE, Davis MA, Myers SR, Adkins BA, Abumrad NN, Williams PE: Insulin, glucagon, and glucose as regulators of hepatic glucose uptake and production in vivo. Diabetes Metab Rev 3:307–332, 1987
Kahn SE, Prigeon RL, McCulloch DK, Boyko EJ, Bergman RN, Schwartz MW, Neifing JL, Ward WK, Beard JC, Palmer JP, Porte D Jr: Quantification of the relationship between insulin sensitivity and -cell function in human subjects: evidence for a hyperbolic function. Diabetes 42:1663–1672, 1993
Bergman RN, Phillips LS, Cobelli C: Physiologic evaluation of factors controlling glucose tolerance in man: measurement of insulin sensitivity and beta-cell glucose sensitivity from the response to intravenous glucose. J Clin Invest 68:1456–1467, 1981
Sacca L, Orofino G, Petrone A, Vigorito C: Differential roles of splanchnic and peripheral tissues in the pathogenesis of impaired glucose tolerance. J Clin Invest 73:1683–1687, 1984
DeFronzo RA, Ferrannini E, Hendler R, Felig P, Wahren J: Regulation of splanchnic and peripheral glucose uptake by insulin and hyperglycemia in man. Diabetes 32:35–45, 1983
Basu R, Di Camillo B, Toffolo G, Basu A, Shah P, Vella A, Rizza R, Cobelli C: Use of a novel triple-tracer approach to assess postprandial glucose metabolism. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 284:E55–E69, 2003
Breda E, Cavaghan MK, Toffolo G, Polonsky KS, Cobelli C: Oral glucose tolerance test minimal model indexes of -cell function and insulin sensitivity. Diabetes 50:150–158, 2001
Toffolo G, Cefalu WT, Cobelli C: Beta-cell function during insulin-modified intravenous glucose tolerance test successfully assessed by the C-peptide minimal model. Metabolism 48:1162–1166, 1999
Basu R, Breda E, Oberg AL, Powell CC, Dalla Man C, Basu A, Vittone JL, Klee GG, Arora P, Jensen MD, Toffolo G, Cobelli C, Rizza RA: Mechanisms of the age-associated deterioration in glucose tolerance: contribution of alterations in insulin secretion, action, and clearance. Diabetes 52:1738–1748, 2003
Dalla Man C, Yarasheski KE, Caumo A, Robertson H, Toffolo G, Polonsky KS, Cobelli C: Insulin sensitivity by oral glucose minimal models: validation against clamp. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 289:E954–E959, 2005
Dalla Man C, Caumo A, Basu R, Rizza R, Toffolo G, Cobelli C: Measurement of selective effect of insulin on glucose disposal from labeled glucose oral test minimal model. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 289:E909–E914, 2005
Dalla Man C, Caumo A, Basu R, Rizza R, Toffolo G, Cobelli C: Minimal model estimation of glucose absorption and insulin sensitivity from oral test: validation with a tracer method. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 287:E637–E643, 2004
Caumo A, Bergman RN, Cobelli C: Insulin sensitivity from meal tolerance tests in normal subjects: a minimal model index. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 85:4396–4402, 2000
McMahon M, Marsh H, Rizza R: Comparison of the pattern of postprandial carbohydrate metabolism after ingestion of a glucose drink or a mixed meal. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 68:647–653, 1989
Singhal P, Caumo A, Carey PE, Cobelli C, Taylor R: Regulation of endogenous glucose production after a mixed meal in type 2 diabetes. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 283:E275–E283, 2002
Beylot M, Previs SF, David F, Brunengraber H: Determination of the 13C-labeling pattern of glucose by gas chromatography-mass spectrometry. Anal Biochem 212:526–531, 1993
Radziuk J, Norwich KH, Vranic M: Experimental validation of measurements of glucose turnover in nonsteady state. Am J Physiol 234:E84–E93, 1978
Van Cauter E, Mestrez F, Sturis J, Polonsky KS: Estimation of insulin secretion rates from C-peptide levels: comparison of individual and standard kinetic parameters for C-peptide clearance. Diabetes 41:368–377, 1992
Barrett PH, Bell BM, Cobelli C, Golde H, Schumitzky A, Vicini P, Foster DM: SAAM II: simulation, analysis, and modeling software for tracer and pharmacokinetic studies. Metabolism 47:484–492, 1998
Toffolo G, Campioni M, Basu R, Rizza RA, Cobelli C: A minimal model of insulin secretion and kinetics to assess hepatic insulin extraction. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 290:E169–E176, 2006
Lillioja S, Mott DM, Howard BV, Bennett PH, Yki-Jarvinen H, Freymond D, Nyomba BL, Zurlo F, Swinburn B, Bogardus C: Impaired glucose tolerance as a disorder of insulin action: longitudinal and cross-sectional studies in Pima Indians. N Engl J Med 318:1217–1225, 1988
Firth RG, Bell PM, Marsh HM, Hansen I, Rizza RA: Postprandial hyperglycemia in patients with noninsulin-dependent diabetes mellitus: role of hepatic and extrahepatic tissues. J Clin Invest 77:1525–1532, 1986
Ferrannini E, Simonson DC, Katz LD, Reichard G Jr, Bevilacqua S, Barrett EJ, Olsson M, DeFronzo RA: The disposal of an oral glucose load in patients with non-insulin-dependent diabetes. Metabolism 37:79–85, 1988
Kelley D, Mokan M, Veneman T: Impaired postprandial glucose utilization in non-insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. Metabolism 43:1549–1557, 1994
Mitrakou A, Kelley D, Veneman T, Jenssen T, Pangburn T, Reilly J, Gerich J: Contribution of abnormal muscle and liver glucose metabolism to postprandial hyperglycemia in NIDDM. Diabetes 39:1381–1390, 1990
Basu A, Alzaid A, Dinneen S, Caumo A, Cobelli C, Rizza RA: Effects of a change in the pattern of insulin delivery on carbohydrate tolerance in diabetic and nondiabetic humans in the presence of differing degrees of insulin resistance. J Clin Invest 97:2351–2361, 1996
O’Rahilly S, Hattersley A, Vaag A, Gray H: Insulin resistance as the major cause of impaired glucose tolerance: a self-fulfilling prophesy Lancet 344:585–589, 1994
Prigeon RL, Roder ME, Porte D Jr, Kahn SE: The effect of insulin dose on the measurement of insulin sensitivity by the minimal model technique: evidence for saturable insulin transport in humans. J Clin Invest 97:501–507, 1996
Doeden B, Rizza R: Use of a variable insulin infusion to assess insulin action in obesity: defects in both the kinetics and amplitude of response. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 64:902–908, 1987
Bergman RN, Finegood DT, Ader M: Assessment of insulin sensitivity in vivo. Endocr Rev 6:45–86, 1985
Hanefeld M, Koehler C, Fuecker K, Henkel E, Schaper F, Temelkova-Kurktschiev T: Insulin secretion and insulin sensitivity pattern is different in isolated impaired glucose tolerance and impaired fasting glucose: the risk factor in Impaired Glucose Tolerance for Atherosclerosis and Diabetes study. Diabetes Care 26:868–874, 2003
Tripathy D, Carlsson M, Almgren P, Isomaa B, Taskinen MR, Tuomi T, Groop LC: Insulin secretion and insulin sensitivity in relation to glucose tolerance: lessons from the Botnia Study. Diabetes 49:975–980, 2000
Jensen CC, Cnop M, Hull RL, Fujimoto WY, Kahn SE: -Cell function is a major contributor to oral glucose tolerance in high-risk relatives of four ethnic groups in the U.S. Diabetes 51:2170–2178, 2002
Li CL, Tsai ST, Chou P: Relative role of insulin resistance and beta-cell dysfunction in the progression to type 2 diabetes: the Kinmen Study. Diabetes Res Clin Pract 59:225–232, 2003
Kim DJ, Lee MS, Kim KW, Lee MK: Insulin secretory dysfunction and insulin resistance in the pathogenesis of korean type 2 diabetes mellitus. Metabolism 50:590–593, 2001
Carnevale Schianca GP, Rossi A, Sainaghi PP, Maduli E, Bartoli E: The significance of impaired fasting glucose versus impaired glucose tolerance: importance of insulin secretion and resistance. Diabetes Care 26:1333–1337, 2003
Utzschneider KM, Prigeon RL, Carr DB, Hull RL, Tong J, Shofer JB, Retzlaff BM, Knopp RH, Kahn SE: Impact of differences in fasting glucose and glucose tolerance on the hyperbolic relationship between insulin sensitivity and insulin responses. Diabetes Care 29:356–362, 2006
Ferrannini E, Gastaldelli A, Miyazaki Y, Matsuda M, Mari A, DeFronzo RA: Beta-cell function in subjects spanning the range from normal glucose tolerance to overt diabetes: a new analysis. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 90:493–500, 2005
Rutter GA: Visualising insulin secretion: the Minkowski Lecture 2004. Diabetologia 47:1861–1872, 2004
Barg S, Eliasson L, Renstrom E, Rorsman P: A subset of 50 secretory granules in close contact with L-type Ca2+ channels accounts for first-phase insulin secretion in mouse -cells. Diabetes 51 (Suppl. 1):S74–S82, 2002
Duncan RR, Greaves J, Wiegand UK, Matskevich I, Bodammer G, Apps DK, Shipston MJ, Chow RH: Functional and spatial segregation of secretory vesicle pools according to vesicle age. Nature 422:176–180, 2003
Stumvoll M, Tataranni PA, Stefan N, Vozarova B, Bogardus C: Glucose allostasis. Diabetes 52:903–909, 2003(Gerlies Bock, Chiara Dalla Man, Marco Ca)